(This is an excerpt from my book, Suffer the Children)
All men are
liable to error; and most men are, in many points, by passion or interest,
under temptation to it.
John Locke
A wise man proportions his belief to the
evidence.
David Hume
The history of the progress of the human
mind is a history of a struggle with its delusions.
Sir William
Osler
A
few summers ago I had the pleasure of visiting the beautiful northeastern
Italian region of Val D’Aosta, nestled at the feet of Mont Blanc on the border
with France. One evening, after a
particularly scrumptious dinner, we were invited to the 19th century
villa of a dentist, whose sons worked with one of my brothers at the time. As we sat on the stone veranda, enjoying the
cool mountain air, the conversation turned to differences between Italian and
American dental practices.
Our
host first surprised me by saying that orthodontic appliances are over-utilized
in the United States and, in some individuals, can result in harm. I remain ignorant on this subject and, though
it was a novel idea to me, I had no reason to dispute the claim. Next, he brought up the issue of silver
dental fillings (or amalgams) which he considered a significant health hazard
due to mercury poisoning. This I
immediately recognized as a pseudo-scientific idea that is well outside the
mainstream opinion in dentistry. I was
put on edge, anticipating the possibility that our host might cling to other
unusual beliefs. He did. He wished to display how his youngest of two
sons was allergic to milk.
He
brought out a carton of milk from the kitchen, and after a moment’s hesitation,
apparently to add a touch of suspense to the demonstration, he handed it to his
22 year old son who was sitting on a patio chair. Almost immediately the young man’s fit and
muscular frame crumpled, and, as his father pointed out, the arm not holding
the carton of milk was limp and weak. I
immediately recognized this as a demonstration of Applied Kinesiology: an
unproven belief system often employed by naturopaths and chiropractors, that
uses muscle testing to diagnose presumed allergies and nutrient deficiencies.
At
this point, I asked if I could perform a test of my own. The dentist eagerly complied. I went to the kitchen with the carton of milk
where I was able to find, with the aid of his wife, two small, dark ochre glass
vials. I filled these vials and returned
to the patio. After receiving assurance
from my host that the “allergy” symptoms would be evident even through glass, I
asked his son to tell me, based on his symptoms, which of the two vials
contained the milk and which one was filled with water. After a minute of alternately holding one
vial, then the other, he confidently asserted that the one on the left rendered
him weak, while the one on the right did not.
Actually,
both vials contained milk. When the
young man was blinded as to whether a vial contained milk or not, his cataplexy
seemingly resolved. Alternatively, I
could have filled both vials with water to see if weakness might have been
elicited by the mere thought that a vial contained milk. I might be criticized for having employed
deception in my test, but had I indeed filled one vial with milk, the other
with water, my subject would have had a 50% likelihood of guessing which one
contained the milk by pure chance. I
would therefore have had to do repeated tests and compare the results to those
expected by mere chance: a bit tedious for an after-dinner rendezvous.
The dentist found this interesting, but
concluded that his son’s allergies must be improving with the treatment that he
is being provided, even though just minutes earlier he gave us a dramatic
demonstration of the severity of his symptoms.
And if one opaque vial filled with milk caused weakness, logic mandates
that the other one must have as well.
But that was not the case.
*****
Doctors,
like all humans, are vulnerable to forming irrational beliefs; lending more
credence to gut feelings than available evidence, making non-existent
associations of cause and effect, falling prey to preconceived biases, and
sometimes, engaging in outright superstition: a belief that there are
occurrences that cannot be explained by the laws of nature, that are influenced
by magical or supernatural forces.
In
medical school, in an attempt to boost our performance on exams, many of us
utilized amulets or good luck charms. A
published survey of medical students from Basel, Switzerland revealed that 10%
of medical students admitted taking good luck charms to their first exam in the
medical curriculum. Other students
adhere to inane rituals involving peculiar diets, preferred items of clothing
and favorite study locations that have no connection to the actual mastering of
the material being studied.
In
residency, superstitious beliefs are even more prevalent. Newly minted physicians are quickly
categorized into two groups: white
clouds and black clouds, with the latter group comprised of individuals who
seem to be jinxed, having the busiest nights on call, invariably forced to tend
to patients who seem to have the worst complications while on their watch. Understand that this is not viewed as a set
of work habits and traits possessed by the individual that makes him more prone
to encounter difficulties (as is clearly the case sometimes) but rather the
result of an unalterable fate; a type of cosmic karma. Once branded a white
cloud or a black cloud, the designation sticks throughout one’s training, and
often, many years into practice.
In
one survey, 22% of residents admitted to being superstitious in general (which
the authors defined as firmly holding beliefs in the absence of objective
evidence), 32% had developed belief systems about their nights on call, and 65%
subscribed to the belief that some residents are “black clouds”. A variety of ideas were commonly shared: that weather patterns can affect how busy
one’s night on call is, as does a full moon; that the word “quiet” must never
be uttered, as certain words or actions may spark the ire of “vaguely defined call gods”. Interestingly, or dismayingly, some of these
beliefs intensified through the years of training.
Check out my new medical suspense:
The Art of Forgetting
Superstition
is a way to deal with uncertainty, especially when one senses a lack of control
over one’s environment. It is witnessed
in competitive athletes as well as in soldiers deployed to the battlefront;
settings that share features in common with the experiences of medical students
and residents: a high level of competition, stress, fatigue, and sleep
deprivation.
Although
the beliefs discussed so far can be viewed as innocent if fatuous notions, of
no consequence to the actual practice of medicine, they betray a surprising
lack of skepticism for such highly learned individuals. This ease with which physicians believe
dubious ideas exposes a pliability in their thought processes that inclines
them to accepting unproven axioms that do
have a direct effect on patient care.
Often, what we physicians presume to be scientifically validated
precepts are actually a hodge-podge of folklore and myth, passed from one
generation of physicians to the next, peppered with untruths, promulgated by
questionable sources such as the cleverly edited script of a pharmaceutical
representative.
Doctors
are not gullible, nor are they eager to be the first to try a new treatment
with little deliberation; in fact many among our ranks are outright
conservatives, reluctant to revisit, not to mention change, any aspect of our
practice. Yet many doctors base
treatment decisions not on available evidence, but on fickle criteria:
following the advice of drug company representatives that call on them with
free samples, mimicking what other physicians in their group or community are
doing, or by simply proclaiming, “That’s how we did it in Chicago when I was a
resident”. Physicians should be able to
research the medical literature and evaluate the quality of information
available to choose the best way to manage a particular condition. But many physicians either neglect to do so,
or they lack the skill to synthesize data into a workable approach.
Learning Science Versus Thinking like a
Scientist
When
I was applying to medical school as an undergraduate student, I asked one of my
favorite professors for a letter of recommendation. I was surprised to find he was disappointed
with my career choice.
“Why
would you want to be a doctor and work as a glorified technician when you could
be a scientist?” he asked me. I was
taken aback. Doctors as mere
technicians? Medicine was science, I insisted. After all I had to master Physics, Chemistry,
Organic Chemistry, Biochemistry and Physiology just to become a candidate for
medical school. It was only years later,
when I was already in practice (and I had read Sinclair Lewis’ novel, Arrowsmith) that I understood and
grudgingly acknowledged the basic truth of my professor’s assertion. The point is rendered limpid by the words of
Carl Sagan taken from an interview with Charlie Rose: “Science is more than a
body of knowledge; it’s a way of thinking, a way of skeptically interrogating
the universe with a fine understanding of human fallibility.”
This
way of thinking is lacking in the average general practitioner who in addition,
is too often oblivious to his human fallibility. There is the distinct risk among us
physicians to abandon thinking altogether in favor of a cookbook approach to
delivering care, incorporating bizarre ideas along the way, adopting a
self-congratulatory smugness as we claim expertise when none is present,
becoming unjustifiably arrogant in the process.
To understand how this is possible we must start with a glimpse at the
long and arduous training process that aspiring doctors must undertake to earn
their degrees and allow them to become fully licensed.
*****
Traditionally,
medical school is divided into two parts.
The first two years are heavily weighted towards classroom learning of
basic science subjects such as Anatomy, Biochemistry, Physiology, Pharmacology,
and Pathology, while the last two years are spent in hospital clerkships, at
the bedside, refining the skills of taking medical histories from patients,
performing physical exams, presenting information in an organized fashion to
other team members, and mastering a variety of medical procedures, such as
venipuncture, intravenous catheter insertion, placement of naso-gastric tubes,
even scrubbing in for surgery. Though of
late most medical schools have introduced clinical coursework earlier in the
curriculum, for the most part the basic structure remains unaltered.
The
amount of material, the sheer number of bits of information that has to be
assimilated in those first two years in order to pass standardized exams, is
overwhelming. One of my classmates put it best when he said that medical school
was like trying to take a drink of water from a wide open fire hydrant. Frequently, a professor, wanting to stress
the unique importance of a concept she is about to teach, will say, “You will
only retain 5% of what you learn in medical school, and this is part of the 5%
you can’t afford to forget.”
So
how do medical students manage to soak up the ocean of information they are
expected to retain, at least long enough to pass their exams, with their puny
sponges of gray matter? Often they
resort to shortcuts, tricks and mnemonics, many made more memorable by their
sexually explicit nature. I still
remember the twelve cranial nerves by reciting: “On Old Olympus Towering Tops A
Finn And German Viewed Some Hops”. When I first used the mnemonic, however, I
had particular difficulty remembering the tenth cranial nerve, the Vagus,
because I would mistakenly replace “Viewed” with “Brewed”; a seemingly more
logical activity for a German to embark in with hops.
Likewise,
to remember whether a particular cranial nerve has only sensory function (S),
motor function (M), or both (B), I still employ the mnemonic: “Some Say Marry
Money But My Brother Says Big Boobs Matter Most”. A good trick, a particularly memorable
mnemonic device, is a hot commodity in medical school because it creates
reliable scaffolding onto which one can pile information quickly and for the
long term. And speed is of the essence
in medical school: we must memorize so
much information that we don’t have the time or inclination to question the
validity of what we learn.
This
attitude carries on with even greater urgency in residency. Now we must know things not for exam
purposes, but in order to provide appropriate care to patients, particularly
when we find ourselves relatively isolated on our nights on call. To some degree the internet now provides
residents with a wealth of information at their fingertips, without having to
stuff the pockets of their white coats with medical reference books, but there
is still a hunger for learning simple algorithms to buoy us in our ocean of
uncertainty. And the proximate source of
these algorithms is inevitably a more senior resident, who in turn learned a
particular pearl from his senior residents, with tenets being passed from
generation to generation, with hardly a trace of the original source. This is thankfully balanced by exposure to
dedicated academic physicians, true role models, who engage residents and
stimulate critical thinking using a Socratic method of discourse and bedside
demonstrations of proper interviewing and examination techniques.
Gradually,
despite the deficiencies in their training, many Pediatric residents, through
experience and study, mature into competent practitioners, able to embark on
their life-long commitment to learning and perfecting their craft. But if they pursue a typical private
practice position they may find that their training leaned far too heavily
towards tertiary care leaving large lacunae in their knowledge of primary care
issues. Paradoxically, a pediatrician
who has just finished residency may feel more comfortable providing care to a
child hospitalized with a perforated appendix than answering a mother’s
questions about the timing of introduction of solid foods. The entire process is akin to training bus
drivers by having them fly Boeing-747 jumbo jets.
As
doctors are separated from their training by geographic distance and the
passage of years, no longer constrained in their actions by the supervising eye
of attending physicians, some start toying with creative, if questionable,
approaches (creativity is a dubious virtue for a physician). Never having fully
embraced the necessarily skeptic and disciplined stance typical of science,
they become vulnerable to unproven claims, myths and, in extreme cases,
pseudo-science. Some doctors start believing all manner of bunk: that child obesity is due to vitamin
deficiencies; that almost all human illness is caused by insufficient levels of
HDL (high density lipo-protein) cholesterol; and that immunizing children is
more harmful than beneficial. They start
making sweeping generalizations based on the flimsiest experience, turning a
blind eye to tangible evidence.
They
are encouraged in proffering advice on matters of which they are ignorant
believing to be guided by a well-grounded common sense. But as Albert Einstein pointed out, “Common
sense is the collection of prejudices acquired by age eighteen”. Some things in medicine are
counter-intuitive. Before advising we
should confirm that our intuitions are correct with research and study.
White Coat Phobia:
It
is time to analyze some of the most common misconceptions among pediatricians,
starting with a rather trivial question, “What should I wear?” Many Pediatricians have shed their white
coats for more comfortable and relaxed attire.
After all, kids are scared of white coats, right? Well, not so fast. There are a few studies evaluating the
perception that patients have of physicians based on their attire. Overall, most patients have a more positive
impression of the physician in the traditional white smock.
A
couple of studies specifically looked at children’s impression of physicians
wearing different clothing. One study
showed that physicians wearing white coats were more likely to be rated as
competent by children. Those dressed
casually were more likely to be regarded as friendly, but not competent. Another study had children and parents rate
pairs of photographs of physicians in different poses (standing versus stooping,
smiling versus stern expression) and wearing different forms of attire. The smiling doctors were preferred over the
stern ones by both parents and children; however, 54% of children preferred the
ones wearing white coats over the ones in casual dress, compared to only 35% of
parents (parents have bought into this myth, likely affecting their
preference). So the lesson to be learned
is that if we want to be perceived as competent, yet caring, we should wear white
coats and smile, and if you prefer not to don the white coat, realize that you
are not doing it for the child’s benefit.
The worst faux-pas, from a patient’s perspective and a stylistic one as
well, was to wear sneakers.
I
admit that the issue of apparel is petty but I use it to exemplify the fact
that almost no one questions the misconception that children are scared of
white coats: it is a belief that is
accepted prima facie. Kids may be scared
in doctors’ offices and hospitals to be sure:
they are often threatening, unwelcoming places where uncomfortable, if
not outright painful procedures take place.
They may very well be even more frightened when the doctor enters the
room, but the fear has little to do with the doctor’s white coat.
Formula Follies:
To
the neophyte, newborn babies appear to have a limited repertoire of behavior:
they sleep, they cry, they eat, they pass what they just ate. Whenever there is a perturbation in what
parents perceive to be the customary behavior for the baby, the usual suspect
is the milk being fed to it.
Despite
efforts by many health organizations to encourage breast-feeding, many families
continue to choose infant formulas as the primary source of nutrition for their
newborns. Formulas are frequently blamed
by parents for a wide variety of symptoms such as fussiness, gas,
regurgitation, and changes in the frequency, consistency, color and smell of
stool. Though true cow’s milk protein
allergy (the most common bona fide type of milk intolerance in infancy) has been shown to exist in less than 6% of the
population, in some practices as many as 30% of formula-fed infants are
switched to expensive hypoallergenic formulas.
In
the past, the most common myth surrounding formulas was the belief that iron
was responsible for constipation and other problems with defecation. Many infants were inappropriately fed low
iron formulas, placing them at risk for iron deficiency and its many
complications (Iron is an important nutrient for the developing brain). Most pediatricians have fortunately abandoned
this practice though new acts have taken its place.
A
little over a decade ago, it became vastly popular to utilize cow’s milk based
formulas that were enzymatically treated to remove (or at least reduce) the
sugar, lactose. I remember having a
conversation with a representative of a large formula manufacturer who tried to
persuade me that lactose intolerance was the most common feeding problem in
infancy. When I argued that he was
grossly mistaken and provided him with actual statistics he quickly changed the
subject, yet his efforts, and those of his colleagues were hugely successful in
promoting these unnecessary formulas.
Within a year or two, an inordinate number of infants (some 30% of
market share in my community in South Texas) were being fed the milk he was
promoting. Even now, I hear many parents report the fallacy that their infant
was diagnosed with “lactose intolerance”.
In
newborn nurseries, infants are switched from standard formulas to lactose-free
formula, often on the first day of life, if the baby happens to spit up on a
single occasion – an event that should hardly surprise anyone. In other instances the formula is being used
to mitigate constipation, and, with many families I care for, the parents
haven’t a clue as to why the baby is on a lactose-free formula. The most
baffling practice, one that I’ve witnessed repeatedly, is that of feeding
infants breast milk (which contains lactose) while, at the same time,
supplementing with a lactose-free formula.
Lactose
is a disaccharide: it is composed of two simple sugars, glucose and galactose,
which are linked together by a single molecular bond. In order for these simple sugars to be
absorbed by the lining of the intestine, to then be utilized by the body, this
molecular bond must be cleaved in two by the enzyme, “lactase” which is located
on the surface of the intestine in a specialized area called the brush
border. An enzyme is a protein that
helps to promote a specific chemical reaction in the body. The names of enzymes can be easily recognized
by the suffix, “-ase”. It is interesting
to note that the only place in nature where the sugar lactose is found is
mammalian milk, and baby mammals, including baby humans, are exquisitely
equipped to digest lactose with only remarkably rare exceptions.
These
exceptions include Congenital Lactase Deficiency and Secondary Disaccharidase
Deficiency. Congenital Lactase
Deficiency occurs when a baby inherits two abnormal copies, one from each
parent, of the gene for lactase, which is located on Chromosome number 2 in
humans. This results in a markedly
decreased ability to synthesize the enzyme, causing newborns to develop severe
diarrhea which leads to dehydration, metabolic abnormalities and almost certain
death, unless the lactose is removed from the diet. Fortunately, this condition is extremely
rare. In one series of more than 1,600
small intestine biopsies obtained in infants suspected of having this
condition, the diagnosis was confirmed in only one case. The average pediatrician, with 2,000 to 3,000
children in her practice at any one time, is unlikely to care for a single
child with this disease in her career.
Secondary
Disaccharidase Deficiency is far more common but is usually a temporary
condition. It results in insufficient
levels of the enzyme lactase due to injury to the brush border of the intestine
as a result of illness (most typically prolonged diarrhea) or surgery. Some infants with Secondary Disaccharidase
Deficiency may need a temporary reprieve from lactose-containing milk until
they are able to build up adequate levels of lactase in their brush
border. Soy formulas, which are
naturally free of lactose, are successfully used in this setting.
For
the sake of completeness I will briefly mention yet another condition that
results in severe diarrhea and bloating due to the inability to absorb milk
sugar, but in this case, removing lactose does not lead to resolution of the
symptoms. Glucose-Galactose
Malabsorption is a life-threatening disease in which the cells lining the
intestine are unable to take in the simple sugars, glucose and galactose. This condition is so rare that only a few
hundred cases have been identified worldwide.
As you may have surmised, a formula where the lactose is artificially
cleaved into its two components cannot be fed to these infants.
Much
of the misunderstanding surrounding lactose intolerance in infants comes from
the fact that the majority of adults, nearly 4 billion people worldwide by some
estimates, are unable to digest lactose, though not all are symptomatic. Most mammals nurse their young until the baby
is around three times its birth weight.
In humans this corresponds to about one year of age. In most individuals, the production of the
enzyme lactase starts to decline at about two years of age, yet symptoms of
acquired lactose intolerance rarely develop before age six or seven. If adult members of the family have problems
with lactose intolerance, the natural perception is that the progeny must have
acquired the same trait; and they probably have, though the symptoms should not
be apparent during infancy. The
widespread popularity of lactose-free formulas cannot be explained by our
knowledge of physiology, genetics and nutrition, and one can only conclude that
it is a result of faulty reasoning.
Formula
manufacturers prey on these misconceptions, trying to gain market share and
maximizing profits by promoting “special formulas” in settings where there is
no recognized medical indication for their use.
One strategy is to attach meaningless labels such as “gentle” or
“sensitive” to the names of formulas that are not truly hypoallergenic. Yet another is to try to persuade parents to
purchase expensive hypoallergenic formulas to soothe colic symptoms, even
though our current understanding is that colic is not due to digestive
issues. Many physicians become the
unwitting accomplices in this charade, recommending formula changes at the drop
of a hat instead of attempting to understand parental concerns, deciding
whether reported symptoms are normal variants of infantile behavior, and
thinking logically about nutritional choices in infancy.
Diagnostic Fetishes:
Occasionally,
some doctors develop pet theories that may contain a tiny kernel of truth, but
are over-generalized and misapplied. I
already alluded to a physician I know of who thinks that all illnesses, from
septic shock to behavioral disorders, are due to low levels of HDL, also known
as the “good cholesterol”. For almost
every medical condition, he advocates treatment with high doses of Niacin. Though
HDL has been shown to be relatively protective when it comes to the development
of heart disease, and may be a factor in the development of sepsis in
critically ill patients, many of the claims of this colleague are a bit of a
stretch.
When
doctors adopt a pet disease, they apply the most inclusive criteria to make the
diagnosis, so that soon any collection of symptoms can be seen as evidence of
the condition. If you make a slot large
enough, you can fit pegs of any shape through it.
One
of my previous partners was obsessed with gastro-esophageal reflux in
infants. GE reflux, as it is often
referred to, is an extremely common phenomenon in babies, as it simply implies
that food that is ingested and reaches the stomach is occasionally regurgitated
back up the esophagus, and sometimes, as any parent has had the opportunity to
witness, back out of the mouth. But
effortless regurgitation is not necessarily pathologic. It can be demonstrated in virtually all
newborns at some time or another if one employs specialized tests. More than a disease, it represents incomplete
development of the complex neural circuitry involved in keeping food going one
way, down-stream in the intestinal tract.
By the age of six months, many infants stop spitting up, and almost all
do by the age of one year.
There
are some babies whose regurgitation is frequent and severe enough to affect
their growth, cause respiratory problems, or manifest other symptoms such as
fussiness and unusual posturing. Some of
these children have recognizable underlying neurologic conditions such as
cerebral palsy. In this case the
condition is referred to as Gastro Esophageal Reflux Disease (GERD). It is important to make the distinction
between the nearly universal GER and the rare GERD: the latter is a disease,
the former is not. The treatment for
infants with GER may include simple reassurance, providing smaller more
frequent feedings, and occasionally thickening of the feedings. On the other hand, babies with GERD typically
need medications to reduce the acidity of the stomach or to promote the forward
movement of food down the alimentary canal. In some cases they may even require
surgery.
In
1999, my partner had most of the infants in her practice on one or more
anti-reflux medications. Though some of
these medications are generally safe – particularly those used to reduce the
acidity of the stomach – others have troublesome side-effects. Some of her patients were taking cocktails of
drugs that included Bethanecol; a drug often used to treat urinary incontinence
in adults but seldom used in infants.
One of the medications she most frequently employed, even in babies with
minimal symptoms, was Cisapride: perhaps the most effective drug available for
the treatment of reflux, but with a potentially lethal side-effect. Earlier in the year all prescribing
physicians, including my partner, received a letter from the manufacturer of
the drug warning that some infants taking Cisapride had died due to irregular
heart rhythms. Nonetheless she continued prescribing the medication without a moment’s
hesitation to a large proportion of her patients under the age of 12 months.
Then my partner unexpectedly left the practice, and I inherited many of her
patients.
Over
the following months I re-evaluated dozens of infants that were taking
medications for GE reflux, none of whom, in my opinion, required treatment with
pharmaceuticals. I stopped the
medications, sometimes to the consternation of the parents, who by now were
convinced that their children had a serious illness. Some parents left the
practice to find a doctor who would continue refilling the Cisapride
prescriptions when I refused to. Then,
on July 14, 2000, Cisapride was voluntarily recalled from the United States
market as a result of persistent concerns over its safety. For weeks I received a deluge of phone calls
from worried parents, including some who were originally resistant to the idea
of discontinuing the use of the medication, now baffled by the decision to use
it in the first place.
Years
later, I worked with another partner who prescribed the drug Cholestyramine to
all his patients that presented with diarrhea.
Cholestyramine is a drug that binds to bile acids in the intestine,
preventing their reabsorption, thus aiding in their excretion. It is most frequently used for the treatment
of high cholesterol. There is some basis
to employ it in the treatment of diarrhea under limited circumstances: when the diarrhea is compounded by the
presence of unbound bile acids in the intestine, such as when the portion of
the gut that normally absorbs bile acids is surgically resected. But the diarrhea we most often encounter in
children is not due to malabsorption of bile acids, but to secretion of fluid
by the intestinal lining as a result of a viral infection. The use of Cholestyramine to treat childhood
diarrhea is not only ineffective; it is highly illogical. But there was no convincing my partner who
had found his pet treatment. Here was a
prescription he could write, something he could do, and his patients were
getting better after all. They would
have even without the medicine.
Of Warblers and Doctors:
Most
doctors develop habits in their approach to patients and preferences in the
medications they prescribe that is customarily referred to as their “practice
style”. When I worked as a pediatric
hospitalist, being referred patients for hospitalization from throughout the
community, my colleagues and I were often able to recognize the doctors who had
previously seen a particular patient by the treatment that had been provided; a
skill I likened to the ability a birdwatcher has in distinguishing the mating
call of the Northern Mockingbird from that of the Gray Catbird.
A
child was prescribed oral Albuterol to soothe a cough along with a syrup
containing dextromethorphan? Why, that’s
the unmistakable song of the doctor who works at the clinic down the
street. A playful toddler presents with
the diagnosis of “occult bacteremia” due to an elevated white blood cell
count? Well, well; this could be any of
a number of birds, but wait, the child was given an intramuscular injection of
Cefotaxime in the office prior to being sent to the hospital. That little inflection in the song is quite
specific for the warbler that runs the Medicaid mill just outside of town. The bird call game lost all its charm when,
instead of a curious sounding chirp, the noise reaching our ears was a
distinctive, honking quack.
That
there are differences in practice style among physicians which can be supported
by evidence is understandable, as quite often there is no single clearly
preferable approach to a problem. One
can argue that these variations are even desirable. What is unjustifiable is the embracing of a
favorite treatment that encourages the over-diagnosing of a particular
condition, placing the cart in front of the horse, so to speak. As the saying goes, when all you have is a
hammer, everything starts looking like a nail.
*****
America
has a long tradition of dubious pediatric advice. In the 1920’s, American child-rearing
philosophy was strongly influenced by Dr. John B. Watson, considered by many
the father of behavioral psychology, whose book, Psychological Care of Infant and Child recommended mothers use the
following approach towards their children:
“Never hug and kiss them. Never
let them sit on your lap. If you must,
kiss them once on the forehead when they say good night. Shake hands with them in the morning. Give them a pat on the head if they have made
an extraordinary job of a difficult task.”
To
further discourage demonstrations of affection he admonished, “Won’t you then
remember when you are tempted to pet your child that mother love is a dangerous
instrument? An instrument which may
inflict a never healing wound, a wound which may make infancy unhappy,
adolescence a nightmare, an instrument which may wreck your adult son or
daughter’s vocational future and their chances for marital happiness?” More than half a century later, Watson’s
granddaughter, the actress Mariette Hartley, would blame the practical
application of her grandfather’s theories for the family dysfunction that
contributed to her life-long struggles with psychological challenges.
It
is easy to scoff at Dr. Watson’s stern warnings, dismissing them as symptomatic
of a less illuminated era, until we analyze the quality of pediatric advice
that continues to be dispensed.
Recently, Dr. Andrew Adesman, chief of developmental and behavioral
pediatrics at the Steven and Alexandra Cohen Children’s Medical Center of New
York sent a survey, called the Pediatric Health Beliefs Questionnaire, to a
sample of board-certified pediatricians.He found that thousands of
pediatricians still subscribe to popular parenting myths and false
beliefs. More worrisome, 76% endorsed
one or more practices that could pose a significant health hazard to
children. More than 1/3 of
pediatricians mistakenly believed 8 out of 40 pediatric myths presented to
them.
One
symptom that is of particular concern to parents, and is thus not surprisingly
associated with a myriad of myths, is fever.
In fact, the fear of fever, which is often accentuated by dramatic
portrayals in the popular media, is so intense in some cases that it has been
dubbed “fever-phobia”. A practice that
has become almost universal in the pediatric community is that of alternating
the fever reducing medications, Acetaminophen and Ibuprofen (using a variety of
dosing schedules) in an attempt to suppress fever more completely.
In
a published survey of 161 pediatricians, half of the respondents stated that
they routinely advised alternating Acetaminophen with Ibuprofen for fever
control. When asked the basis for this
advice, 29% said they were following recommendations from the American Academy
of Pediatrics. A puzzling statistic,
seeing as the American Academy of Pediatrics does not have a policy that
recommends this practice.
There
are several problems with using both medications in conjunction as a routine
for fever control. The inconsistency of
the schedules employed can easily lead to dosage errors with possible
overdosing. Published reports have
illustrated the potential for some children to develop reversible kidney
failure when both medications are used.
Furthermore, there is the unintended side-effect of increasing parental
anxiety with such an aggressive approach.
If the doctor is so keen on eliminating fever, after all, it must be
quite dangerous.
Instead,
fever is a potentially beneficial mechanism, an important part of our immune
system’s response to infection, the product of millions of years of
evolution. Even cold-blooded animals
(scientifically referred to as poikilotherms, from the Greek roots “poikilo”,
meaning varied, and “therm”, heat), such as reptiles, amphibians, fish, and even
worms, will produce, in response to infection, what is known as behavioral
fever. They raise their body temperature, not through a complex interplay
involving the production of hormones and other chemicals as is typical in
mammals, but by moving into a warmer environment: a lizard may lay on a stone
heated by the sun, while a fish will swim to warmer water.
Regular
use of fever suppressing medications, also known as antipyretics, may prolong
illness in individuals infected with influenza A and other infections. In animal models, use of these medications is
associated with more severe organ damage and higher mortality with a range of
infections. So why treat fever at all?
Simply put, fever is uncomfortable.
It is often accompanied by headache, body aches, fatigue, lethargy and
lack of appetite. The goal of using
these medications in children should not be the quashing of even the slightest
elevation in temperature, but to provide comfort. This can be easily accomplished with the use
of just one medication.
Yet,
the advice to alternate medications has become nearly universal despite the
fact that there is precious little evidence to support it. Several published analyses of available data
have discouraged this practice. But it
is wrong to think that doctors and nurses choose to ignore these warnings: the truth is they are completely unaware of
them. They are utterly convinced that
what they are recommending is the standard of care, without ever questioning
the origin of this practice or its validity.
As is so often the case, there is a huge gulch separating available
knowledge and current practices.
Forcing the Tissue:
Another
frequent source of parental confusion and apprehension, the direct result of
poor advice from professionals, is the handling of the intact foreskin in
uncircumcised boys. This confusion and
insecurity leads many parents to opt for circumcision outside of the newborn
period to mitigate perceived problems.
Many doctors persist in wrongly urging parents to forcibly pull back the
foreskin in infants, even chastising them for not doing so more aggressively
and cruelly blaming them for any problems that occur as a direct result of
their own bad counsel. But as anyone who
has examined an uncircumcised newborn can plainly see, it is impossible to
retract the foreskin of a baby without causing pain and injury.
Most
medical school curricula include precious little when it comes to the normal
anatomy of the prepuce, so it should be no surprise that there is a fair amount
of ignorance among physicians when it comes to this subject. Our understanding of the anatomy and
physiology of the foreskin was advanced by a sentinel article by Douglas
Gardner, titled “The Fate of the Foreskin”, published in the British Journal of
Urology in 1949. In his study, he found
that the foreskin was retractable in only 4% of normal newborns, 20% of six
month old boys, and 50% of twelve month old boys. In uncircumcised boys between the ages of 5
and 13, 6% had a foreskin that could only partially be retracted. By the teenage years, only 1% of boys have
foreskins that do not fully retract.
This illustrates the fact that there is a normal, gradual process of
separation between the tip of the penis and the foreskin that occurs at
different rates among children.
The
condition in which the foreskin cannot be retracted due to the presence of
adhesions is known as “physiologic phimosis”.
The word “physiologic” highlights the fact that this is a normal
condition. On the other hand, pathologic
phimosis is a condition in which the foreskin cannot be retracted due to the
presence of an abnormal fibrous ring around the tip of the foreskin; the
product of scarring and loss of elasticity of the tissue.
The
forcing apart of normal adhesions in children with physiologic phimosis can
result in micro-tears of the prepuce, bleeding and, if repeated with
persistence, scarring, with the final result being pathologic phimosis. Furthermore, forcibly retracting a foreskin
with a small opening can result in the foreskin remaining trapped behind the
head of the penis, compromising the blood supply to its tip; a medical
emergency known as paraphimosis.
The
repeated forcing back of a tight foreskin is an exercise in futility that is
furthermore, completely unnecessary.
Adhesions will promptly form again even when temporarily torn apart,
whereas, if left alone, over time the inside lining of the foreskin undergoes a
process known as squamous metaplasia. Cells slough from the inner lining of the
foreskin, the tissue becoming less sticky, and a preputial space is created,
separating the two tissues laying in direct apposition. At the same time, the opening of the prepuce
stretches and enlarges, with the help of tension provided by erections and
gentle manipulation, allowing full retraction to occur safely and painlessly.
The
advice by doctors to forcibly retract the foreskin of babes in the hope of
avoiding pathologic phimosis leads to a self-fulfilling prophecy. Though there are plenty of scholarly articles
and position papers on this topic warning of the potential harm of using force
to push back the foreskin, physicians continue to recommend this fallacious and
cruel act, with many children being referred for circumcision for a normal
condition.
An Alternate Universe of Pediatrics:
There
are countless other circumstances in which the actual practice of medicine
bears little resemblance to what is regarded as the standard of care. It is as if an alternate universe exists; one
where principles obtained through scientific rigor are ignored in favor of a
new reality whose only proof is its popularity.
Interventions develop a veneer of propriety simply through their
repetition. Gradually foolishness attains respectability. Unfounded ideas
propagate, spreading from the mind of one host to another, seemingly achieving
a life of their own.
The
concept that ideas can replicate much in the way that genes do, and be
transmitted throughout a culture, competing for their survival in a manner
analogous to Darwinian evolution, was popularized by the British evolutionary
biologist Richard Dawkins in his book, The
Selfish Gene. A unit of cultural
idea, according to this theory, is referred to as a “meme”, and a new field of
study, known as “memetics”, has been created.
The nascent theory certainly has its shortfalls and its critics, yet it provides
a useful framework for understanding how beliefs take hold in a culture. In the culture of Pediatrics, one can easily
envision how certain hosts, possessing specific personality and psychological
traits, and located in strategic practice locations, can spread an idea to
others (shall I dare say, infect?) in a particularly effective way. Absurdity becomes epidemic.
Hardwired fallibility:
Though
most people would like to think of themselves as objective, rational and
logical, the way we come to embrace and hold onto certain opinions demonstrates
that we are far from that. This has
little to do with innate intelligence; in fact highly intelligent individuals
may be more skilled at rationalizing and defending an invalid point of
view. Rather, it is a reflection of our
human fallibility; the consequence of recognizable psychological mechanisms, of
fallacies in information processing, that can wreak havoc with the development
of strongly held beliefs.
Our
brains are constantly bombarded with information originating from our five
senses. They must process, analyze and
interpret this information, before deciding what type of reaction is
appropriate on our part. There is a limit
to the amount of information our brains can handle at one time, dictated by the
number of brain cells (also known as neurons) available to perform a task, the
number of connections between different groups of neurons, and the speed by
which impulses are able to travel the length of a cell and across the gulches
between neurons known as synapses. Thus,
the brain has evolved a repertoire of tricks and shortcuts to provide us with
an adequate, though imprecise, representation of our world.
We
are not consciously aware of the blind spots created by the optic nerve of each
eye leaving the retina because the brain fudges a little and fills in the gap
with a useful, if inaccurate, approximation.
We don’t require a pixel by pixel analysis of the image of a mountain
lion crouched on a rock in order for our brains to immediately recognize danger
and activate the fight or flight response.
Our brains need just recognize certain salient features to recognize a
pattern. This capacity for pattern
recognition has undoubtedly provided our ancestors with a survival advantage:
the sooner you are aware of that mountain lion on the rock overhead, the more
likely you are to escape with your life.
This capacity for rapid identification,
however, can make us recognize patterns that are not really present. We identify figures in clouds drifting overhead,
and we perceive human faces (frequently that of Jesus, the Virgin Mary, or
Elvis) in a stain on a wall, in the wood grain of a door or even on a grilled
cheese sandwich.Likewise, in witnessing a series of events we can convince
ourselves that there is a pattern where none exists. We can associate events that occur
simultaneously or in rapid succession, convincing ourselves that there is a
cause and effect relationship. Quickly
we formulate a rule or law that describes this pattern. After all, such a rule may aid in quickly
reaching an important decision in the future.
It would be apropos at this point to try to categorize some of the
common fallacies that humans fall prey to in formulating erroneous beliefs.
Bias:
The
word “bias” is sometimes used synonymously with “prejudice” and almost always
carries a negative connotation. For the
purposes of our discussion it will specifically refer to a tendency or
predisposition to believe a particular explanation of observed phenomena. Biases are a sort of shorthand of the mind
in an attempt to make sense of events surrounding us. In that sense they can be thought of as the
most rudimentary and crude formulation of a hypothesis.
The
generation of ideas and hypotheses is a valuable skill; one that humans are
particularly good at. Problems arise,
however, when our tentative hypotheses are not questioned, scrutinized and
tested more thoroughly. The inherent
problem with bias is how we go about handling additional information. The tendency is to give more weight to
information that confirms our preconceived notions, and to dismiss
contradictory information, reasoning that it is either flawed or not relevant
to the case in point. In the same vein,
we tend to remember events that confirm our viewpoints better than we can those
that dispute them. This phenomenon is
referred to as “confirmation bias”.
The
success of science in explaining natural phenomena has been its cognizance of
our tendency towards bias, and its attempt to eliminate it by adhering to a set
of formal procedures that includes testing hypotheses under controlled
conditions, confirming observations by attempting to replicate them, and the
adoption of a rigorously skeptical approach.
One must be ever vigilant for attempts to couch untested theories in the
language of science. This approach, best
defined as “pseudoscience”, borrows scientific principles, often employing them
in an allegorical or metaphorical way, misapplying and misconstruing them, to
support their fallacious assumptions.
Thus
we hear about Candidiasis Hypersensitivity Syndrome, Chelation therapy for
behavioral problems and Quantum healing, as practitioners attempt to blur the
distinction between science and nonsensical speculation. As we have seen, individuals with a Medical
degree are apt to fall for this approach as well, if not by embracing a
particular form of pseudo-science, by adopting pet theories that they do not
submit to the necessary process of skeptical scrutiny.
It
may be impossible to eliminate bias altogether if it is a cognitive process
that is essentially hardwired in our brains.
We must, therefore, at a minimum be aware of our biases and understand
how they may sway our perceptions and our decisions. For example, when it comes to the practice of
Pediatrics I am aware of my personal biases:
I believe that most children are intrinsically healthy and the challenge
of the Pediatrician is to identify the minority that are not; I believe most of
the common problems that bring children to the attention of their doctors tend
to resolve without treatment and require at most, only comfort measures; I
believe that children should be on the fewest medicines for the shortest period
of time, and whenever possible, should take no medications at all; and I
believe that every medical encounter is as likely to produce harm as it is to
generate a beneficial outcome. The
nature of my biases may expose me to the possibility of erring in the direction
of missing an important diagnosis and of not providing necessary treatment to a
child that desperately needs it. By
reminding myself of my biases I activate a self-correcting mechanism; a type of
damper that I can only hope will restrain me from stumbling into flawed
assessments.
Over-generalization:
Generalization
is one road to bias and prejudice: forming opinions about many on the
experience with a few; an improper form of induction. In medical practice, hasty generalization can
have several effects. Doctors learn to
diagnose diseases, in great part, through a process of pattern recognition. Though much time may be spent absorbed in
books, memorizing lists of symptoms associated with a particular condition, the
most vivid lessons are those provided by our experience, caring for a patient
with a particular condition. That
patient, seen during our residency, becomes the prototype for that particular
disease, helping to remind us of its identifying signs and symptoms.
The
problem is that the same disease process can have a variety of presentations
and Mr. Jones, whom we remember from our internal medicine clerkship as a
fourth year medical student, may not have had a typical presentation for
ulcerative colitis. In the words of Sir
William Osler: “We, the doctors, are so fallible, ever beset with the common
fatal facility of reaching conclusions from superficial observations, and
constantly misled by the ease with which our minds fall into the ruts of one or
two experiences”. Our entire conception
of a particular disease may be skewed by our limited experience, helping us
fail to recognize it when it presents in a different way.
The Rooster syndrome:
A
child that lives on a farm notices that every day, just before dawn, the
rooster crows. Soon after, the first
rays of morning sun begin filtering through the cracks of his window curtain. He concludes that the crowing of the rooster
causes the sun to rise. This fallacy in
logic, also known by its Latin name of post
hoc ergo propter hoc (after this, therefore because of this), describes the
tendency, when two events happen in succession, to infer that the first event
caused the second one to happen. As a
matter of fact, cause and effect do tend to occur in close temporal order, yet
often, two unrelated events can occur in succession by coincidence. Alternatively, two or more events that happen
in sequence can be the result of a yet unrecognized cause.
A
child is brought to the doctor because he’s had a cough for one week. The doctor prescribes an antibiotic and asks
the family to bring the child back one week later. At the follow up appointment the child is
doing much better. The doctor credits
the antibiotic for the cure. Actually, the child would have gotten better
anyway since the cough was due to a self-resolving viral infection. The doctor
fell prey to the rooster syndrome.
The human body’s magnificent self-healing
abilities and the tendency of even chronic diseases to have periods where
symptoms wane, predispose us to attribute healing properties to all manner of
ineffective treatments, both conventional and “alternative”. Pharmaceutical company representatives, when
calling on physicians, are fond of asking how their product is working out for
us, in a manner similar, I suppose to that used years ago by salesmen of
leeches and patent medicines. The only
way a physician’s personal experience would be reliable and relevant is if it
were uniformly negative: none of the
patients improved, or they all suffered a particularly troublesome of
disfiguring complication. To be
confident that a treatment has true therapeutic properties, on the other hand,
one must insist on objective data in the form of a well-conducted scientific
study, where a large number of subjects are randomly assigned to different
forms of treatment, and neither the patient nor the doctor know which type of
treatment the patient is receiving.
When
submitted to this type of scrutiny the results of alternative therapies are
routinely disappointing, but so are many “conventional” therapies that were
previously thought to be beneficial.
Even some operations once thought to be beneficial, such as the ligation
of the internal mammary artery for the treatment of chest pain, when rigorously
examined are found to be no better than sham treatments, whose benefits are now
recognized to be solely due to the placebo effect: the subjective perception,
on the part of the patient, of an improvement in symptoms based on the belief
that an effective treatment was rendered.
A Need for simplicity:
Human
physiology is extremely complicated, as are the mechanisms with which our
bodies respond to disease states and to therapeutic interventions. A particular outcome may be due to the
interplay of many more variables than our minds are able to juggle at the same
time. As a result, doctors resort to
wholesale simplification to help them steer their way. This simplification may
lead us to neglect important elements, however.
Thus we are navigating fully conscious of the direction of the
prevailing wind, aware of the angle of the tiller, but neglecting to consider
the size of the sails, the size of the rudder, the strength of the cross
currents and the effect of wind shear.
We
come to favor a limited number of diagnoses for the majority of the patients we
encounter, developing a type of tunnel vision that does not allow us to
consider other possibilities. Fever is
immediately ascribed to an infection, and a child may be subjected multiple
treatments with antibiotics over a period of months before the correct
diagnosis of Juvenile Rheumatoid Arthritis is even considered. A one month old infant may undergo multiple
formula switches before his inexorable deterioration leads to the diagnosis of
pyloric stenosis: an obstruction in the outflow tract of the stomach.
Some
practitioners skip the step of assigning a diagnosis altogether, opting for an if-then approach of makeshift
algorithms. If the baby is fussy, then
give gas relief drops. If the child has a fever, then get a White Blood Cell count. If
the White Blood Cell count is greater than 15,000, then give an injection of antibiotic. If
a child is anemic, then give
iron. Any of these interventions in
specific circumstances may be appropriate (except for the gas drops), but not
before even considering the likely diagnosis.
This simplistic strategy is often utilized by less experienced individuals
who have yet to develop a more sophisticated understanding of Pediatrics, and
are using this leap-frog method to keep from submerging in their lack of
knowledge. Among its users are residents, midlevel practitioners, and
physicians practicing outside of the scope of their training. All physicians
are vulnerable to this tactic when we find that no diagnosis is forthcoming
based on pattern recognition. We are
then forced to think systematically, using our knowledge of physiology, anatomy
and pathology to propose a list of potential diagnoses – referred to by
physicians as the differential diagnosis – before selecting out the most
probable of these and determining an approach that will allow us to zero in on
the correct choice. But thinking is such
hard work, especially when you have a waiting room full of patients yet to be
seen. It is far easier to just do
something, create the illusion of propriety and hope for the best.
*****
If
human fallibility makes us vulnerable to being seduced by unfounded beliefs,
the antidote is to adopt a scientific stance: to question our pet theories and
long-held ideas; to insist on evidence and be wary of arguments weighted on
authority; to reject dogma in favor of a skeptical attitude. Some may find such an approach inflexible,
cold, narrow minded and lacking intuition.
They may argue that the scientific approach is only one of many equally
valid world views, and there is much that other traditions have to offer. But at the end of the day, science works in a
highly reliable way. Cellular phones,
air conditioning, computers, jet airplanes, satellite radio, and garage door
openers all work reliably as a result of an understanding of natural laws, the
result of rigorous discipline in the application of the scientific method. Science may not be able to answer many of the
questions that have troubled humans through the millennia, and it may fall
short of providing the warmth and comfort that other philosophical approaches
purport to offer. Some may argue that
science will never be capable of fulfilling some of our deepest emotional
needs. But whether I’m on an airplane cruising
at 500 miles per hour at 35,000 feet, or I’m about to be anesthetized to have
my gall bladder taken out, I’ll gladly choose science over any other way of
understanding our world.
Thanks for a fascinating post. You have a new reader!
ReplyDeleteThis is a interesting concept in which results in what they accept in their understandings and the like. Though, for medical science, we do need to look into older yet ancient healing methods. The people weren't stupid and they wouldn't have kept using a method that didn't work for centuries had that been the case. Looking into the other forms of healing science and thus adapting the current definition of science very well could change and increase the effectiveness of modern pediatricians of a Columbia MD and so much more. But doubt, it seems, it what is holding us back.
ReplyDeleteI'm actually guilty of believing some dental superstitions, but that was before my Chandler dentist slowly converted me to the methodological way of assessing whether the belief was scientific or just an old wives tale. It helped me look at things in a new light, as not all superstitions are fabricated, just exaggerated.
ReplyDeleteGreat article –Impressive:We think you might be interested to know more about our company,helps you get your hands on world’sbest class customized B2B Contact List, which can make your sales and marketingcampaignscost effective. Achieve Your Marketing Goals and Grow Your Business.Use B2B Contact Lists to get better and deeperinsights. You and your sales team get to hear true voice of your target audience,much more effectively
ReplyDeleteascomparetoothertraditional marketresearch methodologies.Chief of Pediatrics Mailing List & Email List
Thanks for this great post!- This provides good insight. You might also be interested to know more about generating more leads and getting the right intelligence to engage prospects.E-Health Care Lists implements new lead gen ideas and strategies for generating more leads and targeting the right leads and accounts.E-Health Care Lists is one of the global suppliers of healthcare mailing list & email list.Marketing to the healthcare industry, reaching the doctors and other healthcare decision makers is often an impossibletask.Chief of Pediatrics Mailing list & Email list
ReplyDeleteI was diagnosed as HEPATITIS B carrier in 2013 with fibrosis of the
ReplyDeleteliver already present. I started on antiviral medications which
reduced the viral load initially. After a couple of years the virus
became resistant. I started on HEPATITIS B Herbal treatment from
ULTIMATE LIFE CLINIC (www.ultimatelifeclinic.com) in March, 2020. Their
treatment totally reversed the virus. I did another blood test after
the 6 months long treatment and tested negative to the virus. Amazing
treatment! This treatment is a breakthrough for all HBV carriers.